Politeness theory

Politeness theory accounts for the redressing of affronts to a person's 'face' by face-threatening acts.[1] The concept of face was derived from Chinese into English in the 19th century.[2] Erving Goffman would then go on to introduce the concept into academia through his theories of 'face' and 'facework'.[2][3] Although politeness has been studied in a variety of cultures for many years, Penelope Brown and Stephen Levinson's politeness theory has become very influential.[4] In 1987, Brown and Levinson proposed that politeness was a universal concept,[5] which has created controversy within academia.[6][7] Politeness is the expression of the speakers' intention to mitigate face threats carried by certain face threatening acts toward the listener.[6] Another definition is "a battery of social skills whose goal is to ensure everyone feels affirmed in a social interaction".[1] Therefore, being polite can be an attempt for the speaker to save their own face or the face of who he or she is talking to.

Positive and negative face

Face is the public self-image that every person tries to protect. Brown and Levinson defined positive face two ways: as "the want of every member that his wants be desirable to at least some others executors" (p. 62), or alternatively, "the positive consistent self-image or 'personality' (crucially including the desire that this self-image be appreciated and approved of) claimed by interactants" (p. 61).[8] Negative face was defined as "the want of every 'competent adult member' that his actions be unimpeded by others", or "the basic claim to territories, personal preserves, rights to non-distraction—i.e. the freedom of action and freedom from imposition".[8] Whereas positive face involves a desire for connection with others, negative face needs include autonomy and independence.[7]

Ten years later, Brown characterized positive face by desires to be liked, admired, ratified, and related to positively, noting that one would threaten positive face by ignoring someone. At the same time, she characterized negative face by the desire not to be imposed upon, noting that negative face could be impinged upon by imposing on someone.[9] Positive face refers to one's self-esteem, while negative face refers to one's freedom to act.[1][10] These two aspects of face are the basic wants in any social interaction; during any social interaction, cooperation is needed amongst the participants to maintain each other's face.[1] Participants can do this by using positive politeness and negative politeness, which pay attention to people's positive and negative face needs respectively.[7]

Face-threatening acts

According to Brown and Levinson, positive and negative face exist universally in human culture; it has been argued that the notion of face is the actual universal component to their proposed Politeness theory.[4] A face threatening act is an act that inherently damages the face of the addressee or the speaker by acting in opposition to the wants and desires of the other. Face threatening acts can be verbal (using words/language), paraverbal (conveyed in the characteristics of speech such as tone, inflection, etc.), or non-verbal (facial expression, etc.). Based on the terms of conversation in social interactions, face-threatening acts are at times inevitable. At minimum, there must be at least one of the face threatening acts associated with an utterance. It is also possible to have multiple acts working within a single utterance.[8]

Negative face-threatening acts

Negative face is threatened when an individual does not avoid or intend to avoid the obstruction of their interlocutor's freedom of action.[8] It can cause damage to either the speaker or the hearer, and makes one of the interlocutors submit their will to the other. Freedom of choice and action are impeded when negative face is threatened.

Damage to the hearer

The following are cases in which the negative face of the hearer (the person being spoken to) is threatened.
  • An act that affirms or denies a future act of the hearer creates pressure on the hearer to either perform or not perform the act.[8]
Examples: orders, requests, suggestions, advice, remindings, threats, or warnings.
  • An act that expresses the speaker's sentiments of the hearer or the hearer's belongings.[8]
Examples: compliments, expressions of envy or admiration, or expressions of strong negative emotion toward the hearer (e.g. hatred, anger, distrust).
  • An act that expresses some positive future act of the speaker toward the hearer. In doing so, pressure has been put on the hearer to accept or reject the act and possibly incur a debt.[8]
Examples: offers and promises.

Damage to the speaker

The following are cases in which the negative face of the speaker (the person talking) is threatened.
  • An act that shows that the speaker is succumbing to the power of the hearer.[8]
  • Expressing thanks
  • Accepting a thank you or apology
  • Excuses
  • Acceptance of offers
  • A response to the hearer's violation of social etiquette
  • The speaker commits himself to something he or she does not want to do

Positive face-threatening acts

Positive face is threatened when the speaker or hearer does not care about their interactor's feelings, wants, or does not want what the other wants.[8] Positive face threatening acts can also cause damage to the speaker or the hearer. When an individual is forced to be separated from others so that their well being is treated less importantly, positive face is threatened.

Damage to the hearer

The following are cases in which the positive face of the hearer (the person being spoken to) is threatened.
  • An act that expresses the speaker's negative assessment of the hearer's positive face or an element of his/her positive face. The speaker can display this disapproval in two ways. The first approach is for the speaker to directly or indirectly indicate that he dislikes some aspect of the hearer's possessions, desires, or personal attributes. The second approach is for the speaker to express disapproval by stating or implying that the hearer is wrong, irrational, or misguided.[8]
Examples: expressions of disapproval (e.g. insults, accusations, complaints), contradictions, disagreements, or challenges.
  • An act that expresses the speaker's indifference toward the addressee's positive face.[8]
  • The addressee might be embarrassed for or fear the speaker.
Examples: excessively emotional expressions.
  • The speaker indicates that he doesn't have the same values or fears as the hearer
Examples: disrespect, mention of topics which are inappropriate in general or in the context.
  • The speaker indicates that he is willing to disregard the emotional well being of the hearer.
Examples: belittling or boasting.
  • The speaker increases the possibility that a face-threatening act will occur. This situation is created when a topic is brought up by the speaker that is a sensitive societal subject.
Examples: topics that relate to politics, race, religion.
  • The speaker indicates that he is indifferent to the positive face wants of the hearer. This is most often expressed in obvious non-cooperative behavior.
Examples: interrupting, non sequiturs.
  • The speaker misidentifies the hearer in an offensive or embarrassing way. This may occur either accidentally or intentionally. Generally, this refers to the misuse of address terms in relation to status, gender, or age.
Example: Addressing a young woman as "ma'am" instead of "miss."

Damage to the speaker

The following are cases in which the positive face of the speaker (the person talking) is threatened.
  • An act that shows that the speaker is in some sense wrong, and unable to control himself.[8]
  • Apologies: In this act, speaker is damaging his own face by admitting that he regrets one of his previous acts.
  • Acceptance of a compliment
  • Inability to control one's physical self
  • Inability to control one's emotional self
  • Self-humiliation
  • Confessions

How refusals threaten positive and negative face

In their study of refusals to requests, Johnson et al. argue refusals can threaten both the positive and negative face of the refuser (the person who was asked a favor), and the positive face of the requester (the person asking for a favor). Obstacles, or reasons for non-compliance with a person's request, can "vary on three dimensions: willingness-unwillingness, ability-inability, and focus on-focus away from the requester".[10]

The willingness dimension differentiates between refusals where the refuser states, "I don't want to help you" and "I'd like to help." Ability differentiates between, "I'm short on cash" and "I have some extra money." Focus on-focus away from requester differentiates between, "It's your problem, so you take care of it" and "It's terrible that your mom won't give you the money."[10]

When a person makes a request, their positive face is threatened mostly along the ability and unwillingness dimensions. People tend to make requests of "intimates," people they are supposed to know well/have a good relationship with. Threat to the requester's positive face increases when the requester chooses a person who has low ability/inability to fulfill the request or is unwilling to comply (the person being asked has to refuse the request); choosing a person with low ability suggests the requester has poor relational knowledge.[10] On the other hand, choosing a person with high ability decreases threat to the requester's positive face because it shows the requester's competence; choosing a person with high willingness reinforces the requester's choice and decreases threats to positive face.[10]

Choosing to refuse or not refuse a request can threaten the requester's positive and negative faces in different ways. When a person refuses to comply with a request from an intimate, they are violating relational expectations and increasing threat to their positive face; however, focusing attention away from the requester can decrease threat to the requester's positive face even if they are unwilling to help.[10] In contrast, focusing attention on the requester can increase threat to positive face since it highlights the requester's unwillingness. Accepting a request is the least threatening act.

Threats to the refuser's negative face vary along the ability and focus dimensions. Focusing away from the requester allows the refuser to maintain their autonomy while maintaining the relationship; this leads to less face-threat if the refuser has high ability because they can choose whether to comply or not.[10] Focusing on the requester would threaten their relationship with the requester and their long-term autonomy (the requester may be unwilling to comply to future requests when the roles are reversed); however, if the refuser has low ability, focusing on the requester can actually decrease threats to negative face by showing they are unable to comply even if they wanted to.[10]

*Note: the requester and refuser would be analogous to the "speaker" and "hearer" roles discussed earlier in the section "Face-threatening acts".

Politeness strategies

Politeness strategies are used to formulate messages in order to save the hearer's positive face when face-threatening acts are inevitable or desired. Brown and Levinson outline four main types of politeness strategies: bald on-record, negative politeness, positive politeness, and off-record (indirect) as well as simply not using the face-threatening act.

Politeness strategies summary chart
Politeness strategy Bald on-record Positive politeness Negative politeness Off-record (indirect)
Explanation Does nothing to reduce the threat to the hearer's face and is

therefore used in close relationships or when information needs

to be shared quickly.

Is used as a way to make the hearer feel a sense

of closeness and belonging.

Is used as a way to interact with the hearer in a

non-imposing way.

Is used to completely remove the speaker from any

potential to impose on the hearer and only alludes to

the speaker's idea or specific request.

Situation of use
  • Urgency or desperation
  • When efficiency is necessary
  • Task-oriented
  • Little or no desire to maintain someone's face
  • Doing the face-threatening act is in the interest of the hearer
  • Situations where the threat is minimized implicitly
    • Welcomes
    • Offers
  • Attend to the hearer's interests, needs, wants
  • Use solidarity in-group identity markers
  • Be optimistic
  • Include both speaker (S) and hearer (H) in activity
  • Offer or promise
  • Exaggerate interest in H and his interests
  • Avoid Disagreement
  • Joke
  • Be indirect
  • Use hedges or questions
  • Be pessimistic
  • Minimize the imposition
  • Use obviating structures, like nominalizations, passives,

or statements of general rules

  • Apologetic
  • Use plural pronouns
  • Relies on implication
Use examples
  • Watch out!
  • Hear me out...
  • Pass me the hammer
  • Don't forget to clean the blinds!
  • Your headlights are on!
  • Come in
  • Leave it, I'll clean it up later.
  • Eat!
  • You look sad. Can I do anything?
  • Heh, mate, can you lend me a dollar?
  • I'll just come along, if you don't mind.
  • If we help each other, I guess, we'll both sink or swim in this course.
  • If you wash the dishes, I'll vacuum the floor.
  • That's a nice haircut you got; where did you get it?
  • Yes, it's rather long; not short certainly.
  • Wow, that's a whopper!
  • Would you know where Oxford Street is?
  • Perhaps, he might have taken it, maybe.
  • Could you please pass the rice?
  • You couldn't find your way to lending me a thousand dollars, could you?
  • So I suppose some help is out of the question, then?
  • It's not too much out of your way, just a couple of blocks.
  • I hope offense will not be taken.
  • Visitors sign the ledger.
  • Spitting will not be tolerated.
  • I'm sorry; it's a lot to ask, but can you lend me a thousand dollars?
  • We regret to inform you.
  • Wow, it's getting cold in here.

Bald on-record

Bald on-record strategy does not attempt to minimize the threat to the hearer's face, although there are ways that bald on-record politeness can be used in trying to minimize face-threatening acts implicitly, such as giving advice in a non-manipulative way.[11] Often using such a strategy will shock or embarrass the addressee, and so this strategy is most often utilized in situations where the speaker has a close relationship with the listener, such as family or close friends. Brown and Levinson outline various cases in which one might use the bald on-record strategy, including:[8]

  • Situations with no threat minimization
  • Urgency or desperation
Watch out!
  • When efficiency is necessary
Hear me out:...
  • Task-oriented
Pass me the hammer.
  • Little or no desire to maintain someone's face
Don't forget to clean the blinds!
  • Doing the face-threatening act is in the interest of the hearer
Your headlights are on!
  • Situations were the threat is minimized implicitly
  • Welcomes
Come in.
  • Offers
Leave it, I'll clean up later.
Eat!

Positive politeness

Positive politeness strategies seek to minimize the threat to the hearer's positive face. These strategies are used to make the hearer feel good about themselves, their interests or possessions, and are most usually used in situations where the audience knows each other fairly well.[1] In addition to hedging and attempts to avoid conflict, some strategies of positive politeness include statements of friendship, solidarity, compliments, and the following examples from Brown and Levinson:[8]

You look sad. Can I do anything?
Heh, mate, can you lend me a dollar?
'Güey, ¿me haces un paro?'*
  • Translation: "Do a favor for me?" "Güey" can be an in-group solidarity marker, usually associated with certain regions of Mexico; literally meaning 'ox', it can be used to belittle someone and/or their intelligence. Therefore, you could only use it with friends without running the risk of a confrontation. To use it in-group, however, is an indication of friendship/solidarity, depending on intonation.
I'll just come along, if you don't mind.
If we help each other, I guess, we'll both sink or swim in this course.
If you wash the dishes, I'll vacuum the floor.
That's a nice haircut you got; where did you get it?
Yes, it's rather long; not short certainly.
Wow, that's a whopper!

Positive politeness strategies can also emerge in situations where the speakers do not know each other well. For example, Charlotte Rees and Lynn Knight[12] have explored the role politeness theory plays in general practice consultations.[12] They found that, in an effort to remain polite, patients agreed to the presence of a student observer during a general practice consultation even when the patient preferred a private consultation. Rees and Knight concluded that politeness strategies in the medical field can inhibit patients from providing complete and accurate information.

Another use of positive politeness is polite or formal speech such as Japanese honorifics. Again, this type of formal speech can be used to protect the hearer's positive face.

Negative politeness

Negative politeness strategies are oriented towards the hearer's negative face and emphasize avoidance of imposition on the hearer. By attempting to avoid imposition from the speaker, the risk of face-threat to the hearer is reduced.[13] These strategies presume that the speaker will be imposing on the listener. Additionally, there is a higher potential for awkwardness or embarrassment than in bald on record strategies and positive politeness strategies. Negative face is the desire to remain autonomous so the speaker is more apt to include an out for the listener through distancing styles like apologies or indirect speech.[1] The use of negative politeness strategies assume a direct relationship between indirectness and politeness.[14] Examples from Brown and Levinson include:[8]

Would you know where Oxford Street is?
Perhaps, he might have taken it, maybe.
Could you please pass the rice?
You couldn't find your way to lending me a thousand dollars, could you?
So I suppose some help is out of the question, then?
It's not too much out of your way, just a couple of blocks.
I hope offense will not be taken.
Visitors sign the ledger.
Spitting will not be tolerated.
I'm sorry; it's a lot to ask, but can you lend me a thousand dollars?
We regret to inform you.

Favor seeking, or a speaker asking the hearer for a favor, is a common example of negative politeness strategies in use. Held observes three main stages in favor-seeking: the preparatory phase, the focal phase, and the final phase:[15]

  1. The preparatory phase is when the favor-seeking is preceded by elaborate precautions against loss of face to both sides. It often involves signals of openings and markers to be used to clarify the situation (e.g. 'You see,' or 'so,'). The request is often softened, made less direct, and imposing (e.g. past continuous 'I was wondering'; informal tag 'What d'you reckon?). The speaker must also reduce his own self-importance in the matter and exaggerate the hearer's (down-scaling compliments).
  2. The focal stage is subdivided into elements such as asker's reasons or constraints (e.g. 'I've tried everywhere but can't get one'), the other's face (e.g. 'You're the only person I can turn to'), and more.
  3. The third stage is the final stage which consists of anticipatory thanks, promises, and compliments (e.g. 'I knew you would say yes. You're an angel.').

McCarthy and Carter[15] provide an example of negative politeness using the following dialogue from the Australian television soap opera, "Neighbours":

Clarrie: So I said to him, forget your books for one night, throw a party next weekend.
Helen: A party at number 30! What will Dorothy say about that?
Clarrie: Well, what she doesn't know won't hurt her. Of course, I'll be keeping my eye on things, and (SIGNAL OF OPENING) that brings me to my next problem. (EXPLAIN PROBLEM) You see, these young people, they don't want an old codger like me poking my nose in, so I'll make myself scarce, but I still need to be closer to hand, you see. So, (ASK FAVOR) I was wondering, would it be all right if I came over here on the night? What d'you reckon?
Helen: Oh, Clarrie, I...
Clarrie: Oh (MINIMIZATION) I'd be no bother. (REINFORCE EXPLANATION) It'd mean a heck of a lot to those kids.
Helen: All right.
Clarrie: (THANK WITH BOOST) I knew you'd say yes. You're an angel, Helen.
Helen: Ha! (laughs)

All of this is done in attempt to avoid imposition on the hearer. Negative politeness is concerned with proceeding towards a goal in the smoothest way and with sensitivity to one's interlocutors. In English, deference ('Excuse me, sir, could you please close the window') is associated with the avoidance or downplaying of an imposition; the more we feel we might be imposing, the more deferential we might be.[1] It is clearly a strategy for negative politeness and the redressing of a threat to negative face, through actions such as favor-seeking.

Off-record (indirect)

The final politeness strategy outlined by Brown and Levinson is the indirect strategy; This strategy uses indirect language and removes the speaker from the potential to be imposing. For example, a speaker using the indirect strategy might merely say "wow, it's getting cold in here" insinuating that it would be nice if the listener would get up and turn up the thermostat without directly asking the listener to do so. This strategy relies heavily on pragmatics to convey the intended meaning while still utilizing the semantic meaning as a way to avoid losing face (see below in Choice of Strategy).

Choice of strategy

Paul Grice argues that all conversationalists are rational beings who are primarily interested in the efficient conveying of messages.[16] Brown and Levinson use this argument in their politeness theory by saying that rational agents will choose the same politeness strategy as any other would under the same circumstances to try to mitigate face. They show the available range of verbal politeness strategies to redress loss of face. Face-threatening acts have the ability to mutually threaten face, therefore rational agents seek to avoid face-threatening acts or will try to use certain strategies to minimize the threat. It should be noted that in certain situations, an over application of any particular strategy may actually achieve the opposite of the intended effect, as "certain speakers consistently evaluate polite behavior as unnecessary and offensive."[4]

Speaker (S) will weigh:[8]

  1. the want to communicate the content of the face-threatening act in question
  2. the want to be efficient or urgent
  3. the want to maintain H's face to any degree

In most cooperative circumstances where 3. is greater than 2., S will want to minimize the face-threatening act.

The greater potential for loss of face requires greater redressive action. If the potential for loss of face is too great, the speaker may make the decision to abandon the face-threatening acts completely and say nothing.

The number next to each strategy corresponds to the danger-level of the particular face-threatening act. The more dangerous the particular face-threatening act is, the more S will tend to use a higher numbered strategy.[8]

  1. No redressive action
    *Bald On-Record- leaves no way for H to minimize the face-threatening act.
  2. Positive redressive action
    *S satisfies a wide range of H's desires not necessarily related to the face-threatening act.
    **Shows interest in H
    **Claims common ground with H
    **Seeks agreement
    **Gives sympathy
  3. Negative redressive action
    *S satisfies H's desires to be unimpeded—the want that is directly challenged by the face-threatening act.
    **Be conventionally indirect
    **Minimize imposition on H
    **Beg forgiveness
    **Give deference
    *This implies that the matter is important enough for S to disturb H
  4. Off-Record
    *S has the opportunity to evade responsibility by claiming that H's interpretation of the utterance as a face-threatening act is wrong
  5. Don't do the face-threatening act.

Payoffs associated with each strategy

In deciding which strategy to use, the speaker runs through the individual payoffs of each strategy.[8]

  • enlists public pressure, puts H in the public eye if there are others present
  • S gets credit for honesty, outspokenness which avoids the danger of seeming manipulative, yet can come across as abrasive and tactless
  • S avoids danger of being misunderstood by putting intended meaning directly into utterance without the reliance upon the pragmatic meaning
  • minimizes threatening aspect by assuring that S considers to be of the same kind with H, increasing the sense of solidarity and decreasing their social distance
  • criticism may lose much of its sting if done in a way that asserts mutual friendship
  • when S includes themself equally as a participant in the request or offer, it may lessen the potential for face-threatening act debt
  • Ex: a person says "Let's get on with dinner" to their spouse in front of the television: by using the first person plural of the imperative form of the verb, the speaker is able to include themself as a recipient of the order, just like the hearer, likewise increasing solidarity.
  • Helps avoid future debt by keeping social distance and not getting too familiar with H
  • pays respect or deference by assuming that you may be intruding on the hearer in return for the face-threatening act.
  • Ex: "I don't mean to bother you, but can I ask a quick question?"
  • get credit for being tactful, non-coercive
  • avoid responsibility for the potentially face-damaging interpretation
  • give the addressee an opportunity to seem to care for S because it tests H's feelings towards S
  • If S wants H to close the window, he may say "It's cold in here." If H answers "I'll go close the window" then he is responding to this potentially threatening act by giving a "gift" to the original speaker and therefore S avoids the potential threat of ordering H around and H gets credit for being generous or cooperative
  • S avoids offending H at all
  • S also fails to achieve his desired communication
  • An example might be a physician avoiding bringing up the need for a patient to lose weight.

Analysis of Brown and Levinson's politeness theory

Critiques of politeness theory

Although Brown and Levinson's theory is widely applicable, some weaknesses in their theory have been noted:

  1. Cross-Cultural Validity: Although everyone has face wants, there are different ways strategies they use to accomplish these wants or mitigate face threats based on their culture.[17] Some of this intracultural difference is, in part, due to diverse "knowledge and values" within a particular society,[18] but Brown and Levinson argue that their theory is universal. Many academics have critiqued that many cultures use politeness strategies differently than how Brown and Levinson theorized.[5] For example, negative politeness is the norm in some cultures (Japan and Britain) but not others that prefer positive politeness (Australia)[5] and some cultures use politeness strategies when there is no face threat, such as the Japanese honorific system.[19] Ide et al. shows that the roughly equivalent term in Japanese, teneina, has different sets of connotations associated with it from the English term "politeness".[4] (See Cultural Differences)
  2. The Four Politeness Strategies Are Not Mutually Exclusive: Some claim that a few of these techniques may be used in more than one type of situation or more than one at a time.[18] Additionally, a given speech act (of any politeness strategy) can have multiple consequences, rather than affecting only positive face or negative face as the current theory suggests.[10]
  3. Nonverbal Aspects of Communication: Sometimes nonverbal actions speak louder than verbal communication and might alter how the politeness strategy is interpreted or which politeness strategy is used.[18]
  4. Sequence of the Order of Actions: The order of the conversation may dictate whether a face threat is seen more negatively, this may differ across culture context where speech styles vary and conversation styles vary considerably.[18]
  5. Individual Differences: An individual may have a pattern or way of communicating that they have habitually used in the past that others may consider face threatening or vice versa. Mood may also drive how they choose to respond to a situation regardless of politeness strategies.[18]
  6. Issues with Terminology and their Definitions: various definitions of 'politeness' which make reference to considering others' feelings, establishing levels of mutual comfort, and promoting rapport have been found to be lacking, in that often whether a verbal act is face threatening or not depends upon preemptively knowing how the hearer will interpret it.[4] This view shifts the focus from predominantly upon the speaker to upon both speaker and hearer, implying that politeness is socially constructed and therefore not universal, requiring cross-cultural examination.[4] Additionally, a distinction has been made between first- and second-order politeness, due to the appropriation of an English word for a scientific concept: first-order politeness "correspond[s] to the various ways in which polite behavior is perceived and talked about by members of socio-cultural groups", meaning the connotation of 'politeness' for those not studying it, and second-order politeness is "a theoretical construct, a term within a theory of social behavior and language usage", meaning the scientific application of the term.[4]

Additionally, Brown and Levinson do not discuss all types of speech acts in their framework of politeness, including some that are very common in discourse (such as refusals of requests).[10]

Hierarchy politeness

Scholars suggest power differences vary between strangers and acquaintances, which in turn, shape the effects of the politeness strategies. Social similarity and intimacy are other aspects to consider, as these connections create an increased awareness of the other person's meaning and request and therefore minimize the face-threatening act.[18] In 1964, socio-psychologist Edward E. Jones wrote a book on ingratiation and defines it as "a class of strategic behaviors illicitly designed to influence a particular other person concerning the attractiveness of one's personal qualities."[20] The concept of ingratiation has helped spur further investigation into how it power dynamics play into Brown and Levinson's politeness theory. The claim has been made that Brown and Levinson's theory does not take into account the affect unique dynamic power relations and rankings has on the way people interact with one another (i.e. ingratiation).

An article written by Akio Yabuuchi argues a case for a new trichotomous politeness system to replace politeness theory's dichotomous politeness system; hierarchy politeness[21] The proposed system is made up of fellowship politeness (similar to Brown and Levinson's positive politeness), autonomy politeness (similar to Brown and Levinson's negative politeness), and hierarchy politeness.[21] Hierarchy politeness recognizes ingratiation as a way to communicate within power dynamics.

Cultural differences

As discussed in the first shortcoming of politeness theory, academia's main critique of Brown and Levinson's theory is their claim of universality. Many sociologists criticize that politeness theory is heavily based on Western cultures where individualism is highly valued compared to many non-Western cultures where group identity is valued over the individual.[17] One example of this is the Japanese honorific system which is used in even non face threatening circumstances, which goes against the theory that the politeness strategies are used to mitigate face threats.[19] However, defendants of Brown and Levinson's theory reason that people have been socialized so that the absence of these forms would be considered a face threat, qualifying these forms as mitigating a face threatening act.[19]

Politeness in Israeli society

In a series of interviews conducted by Blum-Kulka, House and Kasper in 1989 of fifty-two Israeli families representing a wide spectrum of Israeli society, varying in terms of parents' ages (from early twenties to late fifties), socio-economic status (lower to upper middle class) and degree of religious observance, definitions and descriptions of polite and impolite speech and behavior were elicited by the researchers. They asked for verbalizations and evaluations of modes of situated speech performance, and encouraged exemplification via personal narratives. Both nimus and adivut are translation equivalents of 'politeness' and definitions offered by the families interviewed for both terms of "politeness" showed a positively association with tolerance, restraint, good manners, showing deference and being nice to people, but simultaneously referred to in a negative manner as something external, hypocritical, unnatural. Their analysis concluded that systems of social politeness seem to represent culturally colored interpretations of basic notions of tact, (e.g., face concerns) as conventionalized in any given culture or even speech event type. As suggested already by the semantic definitions offered for the term "politeness" by Israelis, the constituents of "tact" and its appropriate modes of expression are very much subject to cultural interpretation.[4][22]

Critique of face-threatening acts

Politeness theory is based on redressing face-threatening. It postulates that any act can be face-threatening and that the speaker and hearer use different strategies to save face. Their theory does not take into account the times when the speaker intentionally chooses to attack the hearer's face.[23] Examples of face attacks include, "swearing, cursing, stigmatizing, vilifying, etc."[23] Mohammand Ali Salmani Nodoushan has suggested that face-threatening acts be divided into two categories: face-threatening acts (FTAs) and face-attacking acts (FAAs). He assigns the following attributes to face-attacking acts: "are also face-threatening, rely on threat and attack, may be unilateral, attack is intentional, are ignited by self intentionality, are fueld by motivation (ill or good), attack is avoidable, other may be present, may entail legal prosecution."[23]

Strengths of politeness theory

Despite some shortcomings in the theory, it cannot be argued that the Politeness Theory is certainly a unique area of study within the communication field; it is very applicable and helpful in guiding individuals in ways to improve their speech and actions.[18] Two qualities in particular stand out:

  1. Good Heuristic Value: This theory has motivated scholars to implement more research into grasping these ideas or finding alternatives to this way of thinking.[18]
  2. Broad Scope: This theory considers factors that play a role in the field of communication such as "language, identity, relational definition . . . social power, distance, and culture".[18]

Politeness theory application examples

Further research and applications

Although the Politeness Theory originated from the curiosity of linguistics and language forming, scholars are beginning to see its other benefits: its ability to not only help with interpersonal relationships, workplace environments, and beyond.

Business world

One study by Cynthia Dunn observed a Japanese business that required etiquette training for their new employers.[24] Employers were taught the company's definition of politeness; they were expected to incorporate these beliefs into their day-to-day behavior, such as "kindness," "consideration for others," and "deference and respect".[24] However, self-presentation was also a critical feature employers wanted their employees to improve upon. An attractive self-presentation through various nonverbals and word choice would not only reflect the individual's politeness but the corporation's as well.[24] This decision had very positive consequences in the workplace environment.

Through new studies there is the possibility that the Politeness Theory may penetrate deeper areas. For example, maybe more businesses will begin to take on these concepts and incorporate them into their discussion and conflict-resolution strategies. These could be effective in achieving long-term goals. Whatever the case may be, the Politeness Theory has a solid foundation in the field of communication and will certainly contribute positively to the assimilation of language and civility.

Art world

In his 1967 work, Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior, Goffman posits numerous times that with every remark people make, they risk maintaining their face and the face of others.[3] Politeness theory is generally applied when assessing speech acts or remarks. However, a study conducted by Jurgita Sribaitė looked at politeness theory as it applied to written art reviews.[25] The study focused on art reviews in Lithuania in the early and mid-1970s and analyzed the different strategies used by the art reviewers as they attempted to critique pieces of art while maintaining their face as well as the face of the artists.[25] The study was able to identify face-saving acts and all four politeness strategies at work. The author states, "Reviwers usually appear to have in mind the addressee's positive face (the desire to be liked and be approved of) as well as his negative face (the desire to be left free to act as he chooses)."[25] The examples given show that even the reviewers, who hold the obvious advantage over the addressees, cared to save both their face and the face of the artists.

Humor

Positive politeness strategies are used as a way of giving someone a sense of belonging and as seen in the politeness strategies section, jokes are considered a positive politeness strategy. Therefore, joking can be a way of making someone feel as if though they belong. However, some contemporary researchers have noted that humor is complex and not all jokes can be considered polite.[26] In fact, many instances of humor usage can negatively affect face for a number of reasons: the hearers ability to understand the joke is tested,[27] the hearer may interpret verification of the willingness to hear a joke as aggressive,[28] and the hearer can be threatened even by non aggressive humor if it tests their ability to understand the joke or their emotions.[26][29] In a study conducted by Marta Dynel, in 2016, different occasions of humor used in the television show, House, were assessed and analyzed as polite or (im)polite.[26] In reference to the conclusions of study Dynel states, "Specifically, humor may serve politeness and/or impoliteness depending on the speaker's intention and awareness of the consequences his/her utterance may carry, the hearer's recognition of the speaker's intention, as well as his/her ultimate amusement or taking offense."[26] In general, humor can provide face-saving tactics that enable solidarity, but it can also be a risky strategy to use because the speaker and the hearer must be on the same page.

Delivering bad news

When delivering bad news the speaker has a lot to consider regarding his or her own face and the face of the hearer. In 2015, Miroslav Sirota and Marie Juanchich conducted a study on uncertainty communication with negative outcomes. The authors suggest "First, speakers making a prediction may intend not only to inform about a probability level, but also to manage the hearer's faces or their own...Second, speakers perform face-managing intentions by altering (e.g. lessening or magnifying) the explicitly communicated probablility of a negative outcome...Thus, politeness theory posits that speakers use uncertainty quantifiers to pursue informative intentions and also to sugar-coat threatening news to manage the hearers' or their own faces."[30]

The study conducted required people to communicate bad news to a friend regarding two scenarios. In the first scenario the subjects had to communicate a 50% probability that their friend's new car was going to break-down, and in the second scenario the subjects communicated a 50% chance that their friend's stock were going to lose their value.[30] The subjects of the study reported that "speakers intended to manage hearer's faces from threatening news or to manage their own facees from being wrong...speakers communicated an altered (in the case of our scenarios, lower) outcome probability when they intended to be tactful or cautious than when they intended to be informative."[30]

In 2002, an oncologist by the name of Jerome Groopman wrote an article entitled, Dying Words; How should doctor's deliver bad news?.[31] In his article he recalls one of the first experiences in which he had to tell a young woman that she has malignint terminal cancer. He said to her, "Claire, with this disease, a remission would ordinarily last three to six months. A person could expect to survive between one to two years."[31] He found that this type of strategy (bald on-record) deeply shook the patient (negatively threatened her negative face). He now uses different strategies and realized that with sensitive information tact must be used, but also that the patient must be aware of the true probability of negative outcome. Many doctors, he argues, do not find that balance and tend to hedge information, "More than forty percent of oncologists withhold a prognosis from a patient if he or she does not ask for it or if the family requests that the patient not be told. A similar number speak in euphemisms, skirting the truth."[31] This statement reads similarly to the Sirota and Juanchich study; bad news is very frequently sugar-coated in attempt to save face.

Sociological variables

Three sociological factors affect the choice of politeness strategy and the seriousness of the face threatening action, social distance between speaker and listener, the power difference between the speaker and listener, and the seriousness of the face threat.[8][32]

  1. Social distance between parties (symmetric relation)
    Distinguishes kin or friend from a stranger with whom you may have the same social status, but who is still separate because of social distance. Different acts may be seen as face-threatening or non-face threatening depending on the social distance between speaker and listener
    Example: We may use less elaborate positive strategies or we may choose to use positive rather than negative politeness when speaking with family rather than a stranger
  2. Power relations between parties (asymmetric relation)
    We are inclined to speak to our social equals differently than those whose status is higher or lower than our own in a given situation.
    Example: If a professor is working in her office and people are being very loud and disruptive in the next room, she will go over there and tell them to be quiet but the way she does it will differ depending on who it is. If they are students she will use the bald on-record strategy to make sure there is no confusion in what she is asking, saying: "Stop talking so loud!".
    But if they are colleagues she will claim common ground with them using the positive politeness strategy or frame an indirect request for them to stop talking, saying: "I'm working on a lecture and it's really hard to concentrate with all this noise.".
    Additionally if they are really high status directors of the department she may end up saying nothing at all or apologize for interrupting them, refraining from the face-threatening act.
  3. The absolute ranking of the threat of the face-threatening act
    Some impositions are considered more serious than others. Highly imposing acts like requests demand more redress to mitigate their increased threat level.
    Overall the formula for the weight of a face-threatening act is:
    Weight = Social distance (speaker, hearer) + power difference (speaker, hearer) + rank of imposition

Mitigated speech

In his book, Outliers, Malcolm Gladwell wrote a chapter entitled, "The Ethnic Theory of Plane Crashes." The chapter attempts to explain why so many planes that crash end up crashing from human error, and not mechanical issues. One of the most prominent reasons, Gladwell points out, is the lack of effective communication due to the power dynamic between the captain and the first officer.[33] He uses numerous examples of black box recordings in which the first captain hints at a problem instead of addressing it outright. He introduces the linguistic term, mitigated speech, and states, "We mitigate when we're being polite, or when we're ashamed or embarrassed, or when we're being deferential to authority."[33] First officers tend to use mitigated speech when address their captain and this has caused plane crashes in the past.

Linguists Ute Fischer and Judith Orasanu conducted a study with a group of captains and first officers. They gave them a scenario in which they had to communicate to each other the need to change course to avoid a thunderstorm. Overwhelmingly the captains used commands, or what Brown and Levinson would consider bald on-record politeness strategy, to communicate with their first officer. On the other hand, the first officers only used hints, similar to what politeness theory would consider an off-record politeness strategy, to communicate with their superior, the captain.[33] Airlines have been taking this issue seriously and have made strides in teaching officers and first captains how to communicate with each other effectively.

Further reading

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Foley, William. 1997. Anthropological Linguistics: An introduction. Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-15122-7
  2. 1 2 Hinze, Carl G. (2012). "Chinese politeness is not about 'face'". Journal of Politeness Research. 8: 11–27.
  3. 1 2 Goffman, Erving (1967). Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior. New York: Doubleday.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Watts, Richard J.; Ide, Sachiko; Ehlich, Konrad (2005). Politeness in Language: Studies in its History, Theory and Practice. Walter de Gruyter. ISBN 978-3-11-019981-9.
  5. 1 2 3 Armaşu, Veronica-Diana (2012). "Modern Approaches to Politeness Theory. A Cultural Context.". Lingua. Language and Culture.
  6. 1 2 Mills, Sara. 2003. Gender and Politeness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  7. 1 2 3 Eckert, Penelope; McConnell-Ginet, Sally (2013). Language and Gender. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-107-02905-7.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Brown, Penelope and Stephen C. Levinson. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-31355-1
  9. Coates, Jennifer. 1998. Language and Gender: A Reader. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-19595-5
  10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Ifert Johnson, Danette; Roloff, Michael E.; Riffee, Melissa A. (Summer 2004). "Politeness theory and refusals of requests: Face threat as a function of expressed obstacles". Communication Studies. 55 (2): 227–238. doi:10.1080/10510970409388616. Retrieved October 12, 2016.
  11. Goldsmith, Daena J. (April 2000). "The Impact of Politeness and Relationship on Perceived Quality of Advice About a Problem". Human Communication Research. 26 (26.2): 234. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00757.x.
  12. 1 2 Rees, Charlotte; Knight, Lynn (2008). "Thinking 'no' but Saying 'yes' to Student Presence in General Practice Consultations: Politeness Theory Insights". Medical Education.
  13. Maha, Lounis (2014). "Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Linguistic Politeness". CSCanada.
  14. Ogiermann, Eva (2009). "Politeness and In-directness across Cultures: A Comparison of English, German, Polish and Russian Requests". Journal of Politeness Research.
  15. 1 2 Carter, Ronald and McCarthy, Michael. 1994. Language as Discourse- Perspectives for Language Teaching. Longman Publishing, New York. ISBN 0-582-08424-5
  16. 1975. "Logic and conversation". In Cole, P. and Morgan, J. (eds.) Syntax and semantics, vol 3. New York: Academic Press.
  17. 1 2 Mao, LuMing Robert (1993). "Beyond Politeness Theory: 'Face' Revisited and Renewed". Journal of Pragmatics (21.5).
  18. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Goldsmith, Daena J. (2006). Samter, Wendy, ed. Explaining Communication: Contemporary Theories and Exemplars. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 219–236. ISBN 978-0-8058-3959-3.
  19. 1 2 3 Fukada, Atsushi (2002). "Universal Politeness Theory: Application to the Use of Japanese Honorifics". Journal of Pragmatics (36.11).
  20. Jones, Edward E. (1975). Ingratiation: A Social Psychological Analysis. New York: Irvington Publishers. p. 10.
  21. 1 2 Yabuuchi, Akio (2006). "Hierarchy politeness: What Brown and Levinson refused to see". Intellectual Pragmatics. 3 (3): 323–351.
  22. Blum-Kulka, Shoshana. "The metapragmatics of politeness in Israeli society." (n.d.): Rpt. in Politeness in Language: Studies in Its History, Theory, and Practice. Ed. Richard J. Watts, Sachiko Ide, and Konrad Ehlich. New York: De Gruyter Mouton, 2005. 255-280. Ebrary. Web. 16 June 2016.
  23. 1 2 3 Salmani Nodoudshan, Mohammad Ali (2012). "Rethinking Face and Politeness". International Journal of Language Studies. 6 (4): 119–140.
  24. 1 2 3 Dunn, Cynthia (2011). "Formal Forms or Verbal Strategies? Politeness Theory and Japanese Business Etiquette Training". Journal of Pragmatics. doi:10.1013/j.pragma.2011.06.003.
  25. 1 2 3 Sriubaite, Jurgita (2014). "Face-Saving and Face-Threatening Acts in Art Reviews". Language in Different Contexts. 6 (1): 332–339.
  26. 1 2 3 4 Dynel, Marta (2016). "Conceptualizing conversational humor as (im)politeness: The case of film talk". Journal of Politeness Research. 12 (1): 117–147.
  27. Sacks, Harvey (1974). Richard Bouman, & Joel Sherzer, ed. Explorations in the ethnography of speaking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 337–353.
  28. Norrick, Neal (1993). Conversational joking: Humor in everyday talk. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
  29. "Humorous face-threatening acts: Humor as strategy". Journal of Pragmatics. 23: 325–339. 1995.
  30. 1 2 3 Sirota, Miroslav; Juanchich, Marie (2015). "A direct and comprehensive test of two postulates of politeness theory applied to uncertainty communication". Judgment and Decision Making. 10 (3): 232–240.
  31. 1 2 3 Groopman, Jerome (2002). "Dying Words; How should doctor's deliver bad news?" via Newyorker.com.
  32. Leech, Geoffrey. 1983.Principles of pragmatics. London: Longman
  33. 1 2 3 Gladwell, Malcolm (2008). Outliers: The story of Success. New York: Back Bay Books. pp. 177–223. ISBN 978-0-316-01793-0.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/23/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.