Place attachment
Place attachment is the emotional bond between person and place,[1] and is a main concept in environmental psychology. It is highly influenced by an individual and his or her personal experiences.[2] There is a considerable amount of research dedicated to defining what makes a place "meaningful" enough for place attachment to occur.[2] Schroeder (1991) notably discussed the difference between "meaning" and "preference," defining meaning as "the thoughts, feelings, memories and interpretations evoked by a landscape" and preference as "the degree of liking for one landscape compared to another."[3]
Place attachment is multi-dimensional[4] and cannot be explained simply through a cause and effect relationship. Instead, it depends on a reciprocal relationship between behavior and experiences.[5] Due to numerous varying opinions on the definition and components of place attachment, organizational models have been scarce until recent years.[2] A noteworthy conceptual framework is the Tripartite Model, developed by Scannell and Gifford (2010), which defines the variables of place attachment as the three P’s: Person, Process, and Place.[2]
Little is known about the neurological changes that make place attachment possible because of the exaggerated focus on social aspects by environmental psychologists, the difficulties in measuring place attachment over time, and the heavy influence of individualistic experiences and emotions on the degree of attachment.[6]
Tripartite model
Person
Answering the question “Who is attached?”, the Person dimension indicates that attachment to place can occur both individually and collectively (such as with a community). The meanings that result vary based on the “Who.”
When examined individually, places often gain meaning because of personal experiences, life milestones, and occurrences of personal growth. With communities, however, places derive religious, historical, or other cultural meanings.[4] Community behaviors contribute not only to place attachment experienced by citizens of that community as a group but also to those citizens individually. For example, desires to preserve ecological or architectural characteristics of a place have a direct impact on the strength of place attachment felt by individuals, notably through self-pride and self-esteem.[4] People experience stronger attachments to places that they can identify with or otherwise feel proud to be a part of.
Process
The Process dimension answers the question “How does the attachment exist?” Similar to other concepts in social psychology, this dimension relies on the collective effects of affective, cognitive, and behavioral aspects.
Affect
The most common emotions associated with people-place bonding are positive, such as happiness and love.[4] Yi-Fu Tuan, a noteworthy human geographer and pioneer in place attachment research, coined the term topophilia to describe the love that people feel for particular places.[5][7] Negative emotions and experiences are also capable of giving places significance; however, negative emotions are usually not associated with people-place bonding since place attachment represents individuals’ yearnings to replicate positive experiences and emotions.[4]
Cognition
Cognition incorporates the knowledge, memories, and meanings that individuals or groups have associated with places of attachment. Specifically, these cognitive elements represent what makes specific places important enough for people-place bonding to develop.[4] Environmental psychologists additionally use the term schema to describe how people organize their beliefs and knowledge in regards to places and has led some researchers to note familiarity as a central cognitive element in place attachment.[4][8] This idea of familiarity has been used in explaining why people mark themselves as “city people” or why they develop preferences for certain types of homes.[4] Researchers have coined a number of terms based on familiarity, including “settlement identity”[9] and “generic place dependence.”[10]
Behavior
Behavior is the physical manifestation of place attachment and can represent the cognitive and affective elements that an individual possesses in their person-place bonds. Proximity-maintaining behaviors have been noted as common behaviors among people who have attachment of place, similar to those who have interpersonal attachments.[4] Many individuals unknowingly experience the effects of place attachment through homesickness[4] and will carry out proximity-maintaining behaviors to satisfy their desires to relieve it by returning home or reinventing their current environments to match the characteristics of home.[11] This reinvention of current environments has been coined as reconstruction of place and is a notable place attachment behavior. Reconstruction of place often occurs when communities are rebuilding after natural disasters or war. As counterintuitive as it may seem, trips and even pilgrimages away from places can enhance a person-place bond because individuals grow an increased appreciation for the places they have left behind, contributing to feelings of nostalgia that often accompany attachment and the memories that places evoke.[4]
Place
The Place dimension addresses “What is attached?” and can be applied to any geographic type.[2] Many researchers stress that place attachment relies on both physical and social aspects.[4]
Social
There is debate among environmental psychologists that place attachment occurs due to the social relationships that exist within the realm of an individual's significant place rather than the physical characteristics of the place itself.[12] Hidalgo and Hernández (2001) studied levels of attachment based on different dimensions and found that while social aspects were stronger than physical ones, both affected the overall person-place bond.[4]
Physical
Natural and built environments can both be subjects of person-place bonds. The resources that these environments provide are the most tangible aspects that can induce attachment. These resources can lead to the development of place dependence.[13] Place dependence negatively correlates with environmental press, which can be defined as the demands and stresses that an environment puts on people physically, interpersonally, or socially.[14] Conversely, intangible aspects of environments can also promote attachment. In particular, the characteristics and symbolic representations that an individual associates with his or her perceptions of self are pivotal in the person-place bond.[4]
Developmental theories
There is very little research dedicated to the underlying developmental and neurological processes for place attachment, which is a major criticism for the field. Suggested developmental theories include the mere-exposure effect and the security-exploration cycle.[2] Environmental psychologists have recognized parallels between the attachment theory and the development of place attachment, but the attachment theory at times fails to recognize place as a playing piece and instead classifies it as a background for attachment relationships.[6]
The security-exploration cycle indicates that a place can become the target of attachment when it incorporates both security and exploration. For example, the home, a popular object of attachment, typically possesses a safe or familiar indoor environment and an outdoor space that satisfies desires to explore and expand knowledge. This example is capitalized upon by Morgan (2010), who proposed a combination of human attachment and place attachment in a model called the Exploration-Assertion Motivational System, which suggests that the strongest attachments originate during childhood.[2] The model states that place attachment forms due to a cycle of repeated arousals and behaviors that are linked to both places and attachment figures. As a result of this balance between exploration and attachment behavior, children receive positive reinforcements in the forms of connectedness and a sense of adventure and mastery.[2][6]
Despite the absence of a well-established developmental theory and understanding of the neurological changes that accompany place attachment, most researchers agree that some form of place attachment occurs for each person at some point in his or her lifetime,[1] with childhood homes being the most prevalent object of attachment.[6]
Scientific research
Predictors of attachment
Individualism has been established as the main influencer of place attachment (reference needed), yet there is a desire among researchers to create a list of concrete variables that account for differing extents of place attachment among individuals. At the forefront of proposed variables is time dependence (reference needed). It is believed that increased length of residence in a location increases the attachment a person has to that location.[15] Over extended periods of time, place identity can develop.[5] Place identity is defined as an individual's perception of self as a member of a particular environment.[13] Other proposed positively correlated variables are ownership (i.e. of home, land) and social interactions. Some inversely related variables that have been suggested are building size and age.[2][15]
Measuring attachment
Psychometric and Likert scales are the most commonly used quantitative methods for different dimensions of place attachment, such as belongingness and identity. Meanings of places are often quantitatively studied by asking participants to score a set list of places on the basis of 12 categories: aesthetic, heritage, family connection, recreation, therapeutic, biological diversity, wilderness, home, intrinsic, spiritual, economic, life sustaining, learning, and future.[2] Another example of quantitative measurements are frequency counts with word associations.[5] Qualitative research has been conducted with the intention to gain insight into meanings that places possess. Some of the techniques used for qualitative research are free association tasks, in-depth interviews, and verbal reports from focus groups.[2]
Memory association
Memories combine sensations and perceptions to create images that can be used to retain and recall information or past experiences. Over time, memories collectively allow an individual to develop feelings of familiarity that comprise a sense of place. When an interactive experience with three-dimensional space does not match these developed expectations, an individual adapts his or her understanding of place through learning and sensory input. For this reason, places are frequently associated with memories that can evoke physical senses.[16]
The plasticity of memories means that place identities are subject to change. Memories of places and individual preferences for specific places both change over time.[6] Adults tend to focus on the emotions, meanings, and contextual implications of feelings in association with places. Children, however, focus on physical aspects of environments and what can be performed in various environments, which can be seen in the popularity of “pretend” and imagination games among children.[6] Consequently, childhood memories of places are typically oriented around heavily emotional, intense, or euphoric events.[17]
Applications
Disaster psychology
A healthy emotional relationship between a city or neighborhood and its inhabitants maintains culture and positive attitudes despite any detrimental events that may be occurring in that city or neighborhood, i.e. people having an increased sense of security even if they live in a war zone. When forced relocation occurs, refugees experience a grieving process similar to when loved ones are lost. Through reconstruction of place, the familiarity of and attachment to places lost can be mimicked to relieve stress and grief.[4]
Space planning
An understanding of the psychological factors responsible for place attachment is important for the effective development of place loyalty that allows cities and towns to flourish.[1] Additionally, successful places address the needs or maintain the cultural integrities and meanings that communities have placed upon them.[18] More specifically, additions of buildings or monuments[18] and creation of outdoor recreational spaces[3] must be well-aligned with a community’s place attachment to prevent backlash from inhabitants that do not agree with the intended land developments.[18]
Senior communities
Environmental press is often considered with elderly populations transitioning into assisted living or senior communities.[14] Improving the overall community psychology and sense of community can allow place attachment to develop for both individuals and groups.[18] Developing new place attachment is usually more difficult with increased age,[5] and as a result, recently transitioned senior adults and senior communities are popular subjects for research[2] in order to test the efficacy of various community-building techniques, such as celebrations and neighborhood political organizations.[18]
See also
- Attachment theory
- Behavioral psychology
- Community psychology
- Environmental psychology
- Human geography
- Interior design psychology
- Sense of place
- Place identity
References
- 1 2 3 Florek, Magdalena (2011). "No place like home: Perspectives on place attachment and impacts on city management". Journal of Town & City Management. 1 (4): 346–354.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Lewicka, Maria (2011). "Place attachment: How far have we come in the last 40 years?". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 31 (3): 207–230. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.10.001.
- 1 2 Schroeder, HW (September 1991). "Preference and meaning of arboretum landscapes: Combining quantitative and qualitative data". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 11 (3): 231–248. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80185-9.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Scannell, Leila; Robert Gifford (2010). "Defining place attachment: a tripartite organizing framework". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 30: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Rollero, Chiara; De Piccoli, Norma (2010). "Place attachment, identification and environment perception: An empirical study". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 30 (2): 198–205. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.12.003.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Morgan, Paul (2010). "Towards a developmental theory of place attachment". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 30: 11–22. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.07.001.
- ↑ Tuan, Yi-Fu (1974). Topophilia: a study of environmental perception, attitudes, and values. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
- ↑ Fullilove, Mindy T. (1996). "Psychiatric implications of displacement: contributions from the psychology of place". American Journal of Psychiatry. 153: 1516–1523. PMID 8942445.
- ↑ Feldman, Roberta M. (1990). "Settlement identity: psychological bonds with home places in a mobile society". Environment and Behavior. 22: 183–229. doi:10.1177/0013916590222002.
- ↑ Harvey, John H.; Stokols, Daniel; Shumaker, Sally A. "People in places: a transactional view of settings". Cognition, social behavior, and the environment. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum. pp. 441–488. ISBN 978-0898590821.
- ↑ Cieraad, Irene (2010). "Homes from homes: Memories and projections". Home Cultures. 7 (1): 85–102. doi:10.2752/175174210X12591523182788.
- ↑ Hidalgo, M. Carmen; Hernández, Bernardo (2001). "Place attachment: conceptual and empirical questions". Journal of Environmental Psychology. 21: 273–281. doi:10.1006/jevp.2001.0221.
- 1 2 Smaldone, David (2006). "The Role of Time in Place Attachment". Proceedings of the 2006 Northeastern Recreation Research Symposium: 47–56.
- 1 2 Cavanaugh, John C.; Blanchard-Fields, Fredda (2011). Adult Development and Aging (Sixth ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. p. 151. ISBN 978-0-495-60174-6.
- 1 2 Hashemnezhad, Hashem; Yazdanfar, Seyed Abbas; Heidari, Ali Akbar; Behdadfar, Nazgol (2013). "Comparison of the Concepts of Sense of Place and Attachment to Place in Architectural Studies". Australian Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences. 7 (1): 219–227.
- ↑ Othman, Sumaiyah; Nishimura, Yukio; Kubota, Aya (2013). "Memory Association in Place Making: A review". Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 85: 554–563. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.384.
- ↑ Sebba, Rachel (1991). "The landscapes of childhood: the reflection of childhood's environment in adult memories and in children's attitudes". Environment and Behaviour. 23 (4): 395–422. doi:10.1177/0013916591234001.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Manzo, Lynne C.; Perkins, Douglas D. (2006). "Finding Common Ground: The Importance of Place Attachment to Community Participation and Planning". Journal of Planning Literature. 20 (4): 335–350. doi:10.1177/0885412205286160.